Georgian> (Kartvelian)

Mismatch 1: morphosyntax: subject ~ object

Mismatch 2: morphosyntax: direct object ~ indirect object


Mismatch 1
Inversion in Georgian is one of the most widely cited instances of a morphological mismatch. Inversion refers to the situation where verbal arguments which appear to be subjects (on the basis of various syntactic criteria) are morphologically encoded as indirect objects. The interpretation of inversion is complicated by the fact that it affects not just verb morphology, but also the case marking of the verb's arguments. Because of this, there is some disagreement about whether the mismatch is one between syntax and morphology (e.g. Anderson 1984) or between two different levels of syntax (Harris 1981).

Georgian verbal morphology is highly complex, and what follows is a sererely abbreviated account. There are three sets of person-number markers found on verbs (the v-set, m-set and h-set), which correspond to the three grammatical roles of subject, direct object and indirect object. The full set of forms is shown in the appendix. Conventially, the v-set markers are glossed as 'subject', the m-set as 'direct object' and the h-set as 'indirect object', as seen here:1

v-set
m-set
h-set
v-k’lav m-k’lav-s mi-h-q’vebi
1.SBJ-kill 1.OBJ-kill-3SG.SBJ PRVB-3.IOBJ-follow
'I kill X/them.' 'X kills me.' 'You'll follow X/them.
(Hewitt 1995: 129, 131, 133)

However, the relationship of argument marking on the verb and grammatical role is more involved than that, and case government is not straightforward. Three patterns are found:

subject direct object indirect object
pattern A argument marking on verb v-set m-set h-set
case marking nominative dative dative
pattern B argument marking on verb v-set m-set h-set
case marking ergative nominative dative
pattern C argument marking on verb h-set v-set none
case marking dative nominative (periphrastic)
(based on Anderson 1984: 177, Aronson 1991: 244)

Note that while the h-set markers are used for the indirect object object in patterns A and B, they are used for the subject in pattern C. This is inversion. These different patterns are determined in part by verb class, and in part by tense:2

verb class
class I class II class III class IV
series I tenses
(present etc.)2
A A A C
series II tenses
(aorist etc.)
B A B C
series III tenses
(perfect etc.)
C A C C
(based on Anderson 1984: 177)

Class I comprises transitive verbs, classes II and III are intransitives, some of which may take an indirect object. Class IV may be transitive or intransitive.

Thus, inversion (pattern C) occurs in two contexts: (i) all the series of class IV verbs, and (ii) the series III tenses of class I and III verbs:

class IV verb 'love'
mama-s tav-is-i švil-eb-i u-qvar-s
father-DAT self-GEN-NOM child-PL-NOM corn-NOM
'Father loves his children.'
(Anderson 1984: 187)

class I verb 'paint'
v-u-xat’av deda-s surat-s
1.SBJ-3.IOBJ-paint mother-DAT picture-DAT series I
'I am painting a picture for mother.' (no inversion)
da-v-u-xat’e deda-s surat-i
PRVB-1.SBJ-3.IOBJ-paint mother-DAT picture-NOM series II
'I painted a picture for mother.' (no inversion)
ar da-mi-xat’av-s ded-is-tvis surat-i
not PRVB-1SG.IOBJ-paint-3SG.SBJ mother-GEN-for picture-NOM series III
'I didn't paint a picture for mother.' (with inversion)
(Anderson 1984: 165, 168, 175)

Superficially, then, a construction with a verb under inversion looks just like a non-inversion verb with an indirect object: there is an h-series (indirect object) marker on the verb linked to a nominal in the dative case, and a v-series (subject) marker on the verb linked to a nominal in the nominative case. Justification for the claim that, under inversion, the argument encoded as a indirect object is really the subject comes primarily from two phenomena, reflexivization and number agreement.

a) Refelixivzation

Where there is no inversion, it is the nominal encoded as subject which is the antecedent to reflexivization. Under inversion, it is the nominal encoded as an indirect object:

Gela i-rc’muneb-s tav-is tav-s
G.[NOM] 3.IOBJ-convince-3SG.SBJ self-GEN self-DAT series I
'Gela is convincing himself.' (no inversion)
Gela-s turme da-u-rc’munebi-a tav-is-i tav-i
G.-DAT apparenlty PRVB-3.IOBJ-convince-3SG.SBJ self-GEN-NOM self-NOM series III
'Apparently Gela has convinced himself.' (with inversion)
(Harris 1981: 125)

b) Number agreement

3rd person arguments mark plural number on the verb only if they are subjects. In non-inversion constructions, a 3rd plural subject is marked by the suffix -(a/e)n/-es/-nen (typically, only animates induce number agreement), while number is not distinguished for 3rd plural objects:

P’ropesor-eb-i st’udent’-eb-s c’ign-eb-s ga-u-gzavni-an
professor-PL-NOM student-PL-DAT book-PL-DAT PRVB-3.IOBJ-send-3SG.SBJ
The professors will send the students books.'
(Aronson 1991: 273)

Under inversion, a 3rd plural subject is marked by the default plural marker -t (found in various contexts; see appendix), while 3rd person objects are marked by 3rd person singular v-set (subject) markers; 3rd plural -(a/e)n/-es/-nen is not found under inversion.

Švil-eb-s tav-is-i mama u-qvar-t
children-PL-DAT self-GEN-NOM father-NOM 3.IOBJ-love-PL
'The children love their father.'
(Anderson 1984: 188)

Mama-s tav-is-i švil-eb-i u-qvar-s/*u-kvar-en
father-DAT self-GEN-nom child-PL--NOM 3.IOBJ-love-3SG.SBJ/*3.IOBJ-love-3PL.SBJ
'Father loves his children.'
(Anderson 1984: 188)

Summary

An account of inversion in Georgian requires recognition of three layers of representation:

  1. There is a notion of subject versus object, which some syntactic phenomena (chiefly number agreement and reflexivization) are sensitive to.
  2. There are three core grammatical roles of subject, direct object and indirect object. Under inversion, layers #1 and #2 do not line up.
  3. There is morphological realization of the preceding two layers.

The question remains where syntax ends and where morphology begins. In Harris's (1981) analysis, inversion in layer #2 is a syntactic operation. On this view, syntax is multilayered syntactic representation, and morphology is a direct realization of this; there is no syntax-morphology mismatch. In Anderson's (1984) account, inversion in layer #2 is morphological operation. Syntactic representation is simple, and the complexity arises in its morphological realization; thus, on this view, there is a syntax-morphoogy mismatch.

Mismatch 2

The verb 'let go' is inflected for an indirect object. However, the nominal that the indirect object marker agrees with is inflected as direct object. This is evident in series II tenses, where the case marking of direct objects (nominative) and indirect objects (dative) is distinct.

Q’araul-ma t’usag-i ga-u-šv-a
guard-ERG prisoner-nom PRVB-3.IOBJ-let.go-3SG.SBJ
'The children love their father.'
(Hewitt 1995: 551)


Appendix

Agreement markers on verbs.

v-set m-set h-set
1SG v- m- m-, mi-, me-
2SG g- g-, gi-, ge-
3SG -s, -a, -o Ø h-, s-, u-, Ø
1PL v-...-t gv- gv-, gvi-, gve-
2PL ...-t g-...-t g-...-t, gi-...-t, ge-...-t
3PL -(a/e)n, -es, -nen Ø h-, s-, u-, Ø


Notes

1 Both subject and object may be marked on the verb, but only one object at a time (direct or indirect) ever appears, since (i) a 3rd person direct object is not overtly marked, and (ii) in a sentence where one might expect an indirect object + 1st or 2nd person direct object, a construction is used in which the 1st/2nd person are not treated as arguments of the verb. They are treated as possessive reflexives, a phenomenon that Harris 1981 terms Object Camouflage (p. 31).

2 The basic tenses are the non-past (series I), aorist (series II) and perfect/evidential (series III). (Series I also consists of the imperfect/conditional and non-past subjunctive, series II also includes the aorist subjunctive, and series III includes the pluperfect.)


References

Anderson, Stephen R. 1984. On representations in morphology: case marking, agreement and inversion in Georgian. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 2. 157-218.

Aronson, Howard. 1991. Modern Georgian. In: The indigenous languages of the Caucasus ; Volume 1: Kartvelian languages . Delmar: Caravan Books. 219-312.

Harris, Alice C. 1981. Georgian syntax: a study in relational grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Hewitt, B. George. 1995. Georgian: a structural reference grammar . Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.